Wednesday, October 11, 2017

District Court Rejects Defendant's Reliance on PCAST Report as a Reason to Exclude Fingerprint Evidence

Yesterday, the U.S. District Court for the Northern District of Illinois rejected a defendant's motion to exclude a latent fingerprint identification on the theory that "the method used is not sufficiently reliable foundationally or as applied to his case." 1/ The court also precluded, as too "distracting," cross-examination of the FBI latent-print examiner about the FBI's infamous error in apprehending of Brandon Mayfield as the Madrid train bomber.

I. "Foundational Validity"

The "foundational validity" challenge came out of the pages of the 2016 PCAST Report. 2/ The PCAST Report seems to equate what it calls "foundational validity" for subjective pattern-matching methods to multiple "black box studies" demonstrating false-positive error probabilities of 5% or less, and it argues that Federal Rule of Evidence 702 requires such a showing of validity.

That this challenge would fail is unsurprising. According to the Court of Appeals for the Seventh Circuit, latent print analysis need not be scientifically valid to be admissible. Furthermore, even if the Seventh Circuit were to reconsider this questionable approach to the admissibility of applications of what the FBI and DHS call "the science of fingerprinting," the PCAST Report concludes that latent print comparisons have foundational scientific validity as defined above.

A. The Seventh Circuit Opinion in Herrera

Scientific validity is not a foundational requirement in the legal framework applied to latent print identification by the Court of Appeals for the Seventh Circuit. In United States v. Herrera, 3/ Judge Richard Posner 4/ observed that "the courts have frequently rebuffed" any "frontal assault on the use of fingerprint evidence in litigation." 5/ Analogizing expert comparisons of fingerprints to "an opinion offered by an art expert asked whether an unsigned painting was painted by the known painter of another painting" and even to eyewitness identifications, 6/ the court held these comparisons admissible because "expert evidence is not limited to 'scientific' evidence," 7/ the examiner was "certified as a latent print examiner by the International Association for Identification," 8/ and "errors in fingerprint matching by expert examiners appear to be very rare." 9/ To reach the last -- and most important -- conclusion, the court relied on the lack of cases of fingerprinting errors within a set of DNA-based exonerations (without indicating how often fingerprints were introduced in those cases), and its understanding that the "probability of two people in the world having identical fingerprints ... appears to be extremely low." 10/

B. False-positive Error Rates in Bonds

In the new district court case of United States v. Bonds, Judge Sara Ellis emphasized the court of appeals' willingness to afford district courts "wide latitude in performing [the] gate-keeping function." Following Herrara (as she had to), she declined to require "scientific validity" for fingerprint comparisons. 11/ This framework deflects or rejects most of the PCAST report's legal reasoning about the need for scientific validation of all pattern-matching methods in criminalistics. But even if "foundational validity" were required, the PCAST Report -- while far much more skeptical of latent print work than was the Herrera panel -- is not so skeptical as to maintain that latent print identification is scientifically invalid. Judge Ellis quoted the PCAST Report's conclusion that "latent fingerprint analysis is a foundationally valid subjective methodology—albeit with a false positive rate that is substantial and is likely to be higher than expected by many jurors based on longstanding claims about the infallibility of fingerprint analysis."

Bonds held that the "higher than expected" error rates were not so high as to change the Herrera outcome for nonscientific evidence. Ignoring other research into the validity of latent-print examinations, Judge Ellis wrote that "[a]n FBI study published in 2011 reported a false positive rate (the rate at which the method erroneously called a match between a known and latent print) of 1 in 306, while a 2014 Miami-Dade Police Department Forensic Services Bureau study had a false positive rate of 1 in 18."

Two problems with the sentence are noteworthy. First, it supplies an inaccurate definition of a false positive rate. "[T]he rate at which the method erroneously called a match between a known and latent print" would seem to be an overall error rate for positive associations (matches) in the sample of prints and examiners who were studied. For example, if the experiment used 50 different-source pairs of prints and 50 same-source pairs, and if the examiners declared 5 matches for the different-sources and 5 for the same-source pairs, the erroneous matches are 5 out of 100, for an error rate of 5%. However, the false-positive rate is the proportion of positive associations reported for different-source prints. When comparing the 50 different-source pairs, the examiners erred in 5 instances, for a false-positive rate of 5/50 = 10%. In the 50 same-source pairs, there were no opportunities for a false negative. Thus, the standard definition of a false-positive error rate gives the estimate of 0.1 for the false-positive probability. This definition makes sense because none of the same-source pairs in the sample can contribute to false-positive errors.

Second, the sentence misstates the false positive rates reported in the two studies. Instead of "1 in 306," the 2011 Noblis-FBI experiment found that "[s]ix false positives occurred among 4,083 VID [value for identification] comparisons of nonmated pairs ... ." 12/ In other words (or numbers), the reported false-positive rate (for an examiner without the verification-by-another-examiner step) was 6/4083 = 1/681. This is the only false-positive rate in the body of the study. An online supplement to the article includes "a 95% confidence interval of 0.06% to 0.3% [1 in 1668 to 1 in 333]." 13/ A table in the supplement also reveals that, excluding conclusions of "inconclusive" from the denominator, as is appropriate from the standpoint of judges or jurors, the rate is 6/3628, which corresponds to 1 in 605.

Likewise, the putative rate of 1/18 does not appear in the unpublished Miami-Dade study. A table in the report to a funding agency states that the "False Positive Rate" was 4.2% "Without Inconclusives."14/This percentage corresponds to 1 in 24.

So where did the court get its numbers? They apparently came from a gloss in the PCAST Report. That report gives an upper (but not a lower) bound on the false-positive rates that would be seen if the studies used an enormous number of random samples of comparisons (instead of just one). Bending over backwards to avoid incorrect decisions against defendants, PCAST stated that the Noblis-FBI experiment indicated that "the rate could be as high as 1 error in 306 cases" and that the numbers in the Miami-Dade study admit of an error rate that "could be as high as 1 error in 18 cases." 15/ Of course, the error rates in the hypothetical infinite population could be even higher. Or they could be lower.

III. Discussing Errors at Trial

The PCAST Report accepts the longstanding view that traces of the patterns in friction ridge skin can be used to associate latent prints that contain sufficient detail with known prints. But it opens the door to arguments about the possibility of false positives. Bonds wanted to confine the analyst to presenting the matching features or, alternatively, to declare a match but add that the "level of certainty of a purported match is limited by the most conservative reported false positive rate in an appropriately designed empirical study thus far (i.e., the 1 in 18 false positive rate from the 2014 Miami-Dade study)."

Using a probability of 1 in 18 to describe the "level of certainty" for the average positive association made by examiners like those studied to date seems "ridiculous." Cherry-picking a distorted number from a single study is hardly sound reasoning. And even if 1/18 were the best estimate of the false-positive probability that can be derived from the totality of the scientific research, applying it explain the "level of certainty" one should have that the examiner's conclusion would not be straightforward. For one thing, the population-wide false-positive probability is not the probability that a given positive finding is false! Three distinct probabilities come into play. 16/ Explaining the real meaning of an estimate of the false-positive probability from PCAST's preferred "black-box" studies in court will be challenging for lawyers and criminalists alike. Merely to state that a number like 1/18 goes to "the weight of the evidence" and can be explored "on cross examination," as Judge Ellis did, is to sweep this problem under the proverbial rug -- or to put it aside for another day.

NOTES
  1. United States v. Myshawn Bonds, No. 15 CR 573-2 (N.D. Ill. Oct. 10, 2017).
  2. Executive Office of the President, President’s Council of Advisors on Science and Technology, Report to the President: Forensic Science in Criminal Courts: Ensuring Scientific Validity of Feature-Comparison Methods, Sept. 2016).
  3. 704 F.3d 480 (7th Cir. 2013).
  4. For remarks on another opinion from the judge, see Judge Richard Posner on DNA Evidence: Missing the Trees for the Forest?, Forensic Sci., Stat. & L., July 19, 2014, http://for-sci-law.blogspot.com/2014/07/judge-richard-posner-on-dna-evidence.html. 
  5. Herrera, 704 F.3d at 484.
  6. Id. at 485-86.
  7. Id. at 486.
  8. Id.
  9. Id. at 487.
  10. Id.
  11. Judge Ellis stated that she "agree[d] with Herrera's broader reading of Rule 702's reliability requirement."
  12. Bradford T. Ulery, R. Austin Hicklin, JoAnn Buscaglia, & Maria Antonia Roberts, Accuracy and Reliability of Forensic Latent Fingerprint Decisions, 108(19) Proc. Nat’l Acad. Sci (USA) 7733-7738 (2011).
  13. Available at http://www.pnas.org/content/suppl/2011/04/19/1018707108.DCSupplemental/Appendix.pdf.
  14. Igor Pacheco, Brian Cerchiai, Stephanie Stoiloff, Miami-Dade Research Study for the Reliability of the ACE-V Process: Accuracy & Precision in Latent Fingerprint Examinations, Dec. 2014, at 53 tbl. 4.
  15. PCAST Report, supra note 2, at 94-95.
  16. The False-Positive Fallacy in the First Opinion to Discuss the PCAST Report, Forensic Sci., Stat. & L., November 3, 2016, http://for-sci-law.blogspot.com/2016/11/the-false-positive-fallacy-in-first.html.

No comments:

Post a Comment